The Third Person Pronoun and Demonstrative Resolution in Mandarin Chinese and Discourse Prominence

Main Article Content

Lin Zhu

Abstract

Introduction: The third person pronoun and demonstrative resolution are influenced by multiple factors in Mandarin Chinese. The factors influencing reference choice and the relations between discourse prominence and pronoun resolution are discussed. It suggests discourse prominence is an essential organizational principle in reference resolution. The factors that influence pronoun resolution in Mandarin Chinese might be studied and explained from the perspective of discourse prominence.


Methodology: A formal operational scheme with conditional inference recursive partitioning tree and random forest analysis analyzing discourse data collected from a picture-sequence-based narrative elicitation method is provided.


Results: The results confirmed the idea that the discourse prominence-lending cues, including thematic role, animacy, grammatic role, topic, referential distance, and mentioned number, influence pronoun resolution in Mandarin Chinese. Animacy and reference distance might be involved in the important variances, and reference distance relates to topic maintenance, discourse dynamicity, and structural attracting.


Conclusion: The findings demonstrated that the discourse prominence-lending cues influence pronoun resolution in Mandarin Chinese. The third person pronouns often signal topic maintenance and high discourse prominence, while demonstratives often signal topic shift and low discourse prominence. Moreover, demonstratives often signal focus reinforcement. Therefore, topic and focus are also considered as the two crucial elements affecting pronoun resolution in Mandarin Chinese.

Article Details

How to Cite
Zhu , L. (2023). The Third Person Pronoun and Demonstrative Resolution in Mandarin Chinese and Discourse Prominence. Journal of Contemporary Language Research, 2(1), 57–67. https://doi.org/10.58803/jclr.2023.386107.1008
Section
Research Articles

References

Arnold, J. E. (2001). The effect of thematic roles on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes, 31(2), 137-162. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950DP3102_02

Chafe, W. L. (1980). The Pear stories: Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic aspects of narrative production. Ablex, Norwood, NJ. https://philpapers.org/rec/CHATPS

Chambers, C. G., & Smyth, R. (1998). Structural parallelism and discourse coherence: A test of centering theory. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(4), 593-608. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2575

Chen, Ping. (1986). Referent introducing and tracking in Chinese narratives. Ph.D. Dissertation, The University of California. https://linguistics.ucla.edu/images/stories/Chen.1986.pdf

Colonna, S., Schimke, S., & Hemforth, B. (2012). Information structure effects on anaphora resolution in German and French: A crosslinguistic study of pronoun resolution. Linguistics, 50(5), 991-1013. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2012-0031

Comrie, B. (1997, January). Pragmatic binding: Demonstratives as anaphors in Dutch [Paper presentation]. The 23rd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, General Session and Parasession on Pragmatics and Grammatical Structure, Berkeley, CA, USA. pp. 50-61. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v23i1.1281

Dahl, O., & Fraurud, K. (1996). Animacy in grammar and discourse. In T. Fretheim & Gundel, J. K (Eds.), Reference and referent accessibility (pp. 47-64). Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.38.04dah

Diessel, H. (2019). Deixis and demonstratives. In C. Maienborn, K. von Heusinger, & P. Portner, (Eds.), Semantics - interfaces (pp. 463-493). De Gruyter Mouton, Berlin, Boston. https://holgerdiessel.uni jena.de/Deixis%20and%20demonstratives.pdf

Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. E. (1990). Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 430-445. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.430

Givón, T. (1983). Topic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross-language study. John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.3

Gordon, P. C., Grosz, B. J., & Gilliom, L. A. (1993). Pronouns, names and the centering of attention in discourse. Cognitive Science, 17, 311-347. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1703_1

Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69(2), 274-307. https://doi.org/10.2307/416535

Himmelmann, N. P., & Primus, B. (2015). Prominence beyond prosody is a first approximation. In A. De Dominicis (Ed.), Ps-prominences: Prominences in linguistics (pp. 38-58). Proceedings of the International Conference, Disucom Press. http://kups.ub.uni-koeln.de/id/eprint/24935

Hint, H., Nahkola, T., & Pajusalu, R. (2020). Pronouns as referential devices in Estonian, Finnish, and Russian. Journal of Pragmatics, 155, 43-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2019.10.002

Hobbs, J. R. (1979). Coherence and coreference. Cognitive science, 3(1), 67-90. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0301_4

Hothorn, T., Hornik, K., & Zeileis, A. (2006). Unbiased recursive partitioning: A conditional inference framework. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 15(3), 651-674. https://doi.org/10.1198/106186006X133933

Kaiser, E. (2013). Looking beyond personal pronouns and beyond English: Typological and computational complexity in reference resolution. Theoretical Linguistics, 39(1-2), 109-122. https://doi.org/10.1515/tl-2013-0007

Kaiser, E., & Trueswell, J. C. (2008). Interpreting pronouns and demonstratives in Finnish: Evidence for a form-specific approach to reference resolution. Language Cognition Process, 23(5), 709-748. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701771220

Kibrik, A. A. (1996). Anaphora in Russian narrative prose: A cognitive calculative account. In B. Fox (Ed.), Studies in Anaphora. John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Schumacher, P. B., Backhaus, J., & Dangl, M. (2015). Backward and forward-looking potential of anaphors. Frontier of Psychology, 6, 17-46. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01746

Schumacher, P. B., Dangl, M., & Uzun, E. (2016). Thematic role as prominence cue during pronoun resolution in German. In A. Holler & K. Suckow (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on anaphora resolution (pp. 121-47). de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110464108

Stevenson, R. J., Crawley, R. A., & Kleinman, D. (1994). Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and cognitive processes, 9(4), 519-548. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969408402130

Strobl, C., Malley, J., & Tutz, G. (2009). An introduction to recursive partitioning: Rationale, application, and characteristics of classification and regression trees, bagging, and random forests. Psychological Methods, 14(4), 323-348. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0016973

Wang, Li. (1989). The history of Chinese. The Commercial Press.